SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.24 issue2Conceptual convergences between implicit learning theories and the Evolutionary PsychologyThe need for an ecological approach within the study and comprehension of cognitive processes author indexsubject indexarticles search
Home Pagealphabetic serial listing  

Interdisciplinaria

Print version ISSN 0325-8203On-line version ISSN 1668-7027

Interdisciplinaria vol.24 no.2 Buenos Aires Dec. 2007

 

ARTÍCULOS

 

La investigación básica con animales fortalece la ciencia y la práctica de la Psicología*

 

Basic animal research strengthens the science and practice of Psychology

 

 

J. Bruce Overmier**

* Versión revisada de una conferencia presentada en la XI Reunión de la Asociación Argentina de Ciencias del Comportamiento. Mendoza &- República Argentina. Versión traducida del inglés al español por María Cristina Richaud de Minzi.
** PhD en Psicología. Profesor de Psicología del Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota. Minneapolis, United States. E-Mail: psyjbo@umn.edu

 

 


RESUMEN

Este artículo presenta una revisión de la utilización de modelos animales en los orígenes de la ciencia de la salud mental y provee un ejemplo de mediados del siglo pasado, acerca del desarrollo de una importante y efectiva terapia a partir de la investigación con modelos animales, haciendo notar que muchos clínicos no conocen esta historia. A continuación esta revisión discute lo adecuado de la estructura de un modelo animal, presentando muchas contribuciones de estos modelos a nuestra ciencia, incluyendo algunas que constituyeron aplicaciones inesperadas surgidas de la investigación con animales, que no se realizaron con objetivos aplicativos. Se discute además un ejemplo contemporáneo de la investigación con animales, que generó aplicaciones potenciales en pacientes. Finalmente, se presentan las implicaciones para la ciencia, la práctica y la docencia.

Palabras clave: Psicología Experimental - Práctica clínica - Temores - Depresión - Memoria - Psicología animal.


ABSTRACT

The formal study of animals as a way to gain insight into human behavior might be claimed to begin with Meyer in the 19th Century. Meyer’s understanding of the potential power of the comparative approach has not been shared by many contemporary clinical practitioners even in this day. At the same time, Wolpe became dissatisfied with the ineffectiveness of the then available psychotherapies for treating clients with phobias. Wolpe undertook his researches using cats, first inducing fears and then seeking ways to reduce and eliminate those fears. The most effective way to reduce fears in cats had two components: (1) inducing a state that was incompatible with fear by feeding the animals, and (2) while in this fear-incompatible state, presenting and extinguishing, little-by-little, the fear eliciting stimulus. He generalized this to patients and developed what we now call systematic desensitization for the treatment of phobias. Pavlov was the first experimental animal researcher to use the modeling process to study how findings from animals can be used to study psychological processes in humans (including dysfunctional ones) and to test therapeutic treatments for humans based on tests in animals. The behavior labeled experimental neuroses didn’t occur in just one animal but in virtually all animals subjected to the procedure of increasingly difficult discriminations. Shenger-Krestonikova demonstrated that neuroses were likely the natural, lawful consequence of particular forms of challenge to the animal. Another of Pavlov’s associates, Krasnogorsky went on to show that exactly the same conditioning operations and environmental challenges in children yielded the same neurotic consequences. There are two important messages here for us: the first is the illustration of the nature of the modeling process in seeking parallels of causal chains between systems. The second is the demonstration that neurotic behaviors are not the result of abnormal disease states but rather the natural consequences of specific abnormal environmental challenges. Overmier and Seligman’s discovery in dogs of learned helplessness was later extended to understanding reactive depression. They focused on the uncontrollability of events that characterize classical conditioning by exposing dogs to a series of unpredictable and uncontrollable aversive events. They found that extended experience with uncontrollable traumatic events induced a syndrome of deficits. The syndrome was composed of three major deficits: (1) Behavioral (they where not motivated to initiate responding), (2) Psychological / Cognitive (they did not learn to associate actions and outcomes), and (3) Emotional (they were passive in the face of pain). The additional features included deficits in immune function, increased vulnerability to gastrointestinal disturbances, and dramatic alterations / depletions in brain neurochemistry. The nature of memory has long fascinated psychologists since the 19th Century researches of Ebbinghaus. Trapold and Overmier developed a discriminative choice task paradigm in which the rewards for each kind of correct response were unique to that kind of correct choice. When they compared the results of the common outcomes method to those of the differential outcomes method, they found dramatic effects that they called the differential outcomes effect. The differential outcome procedure yields faster learning, learning to a higher asymptote, and more persistent memory during the delay between the cues and the choices, and resistance to disruption during the delay. It turns out that this effect of differential outcomes training on learning, memory, and performance is a very general finding across a range of species (including humans of all ages). These results have important potential for helping learning disabled and memory impaired persons. The research examples were meant to convey the message. That message is that contemporary basic science research with animals on fundamental mechanisms continues to produce results that are important and likely helpful to practitioners.

Keywords: Experimental Psychology - Clinical practice - Fear - Depression - Memory - Animal Psychology.


 

 

Texto completo

 

Referencias Bibliográficas

1 Harlow, H.F. & Suomi, S.J. (1970). Nature of love: Simplified. American Psychologist, 25(2), 161-168.         [ Links ]

2 Hochhalter, A.K., Sweeney, W.A., Bakke, B.L., Holub, R.J. & Overmier, J.B. (2000). Improving face recognition in alcohol dementia. Clinical Gerontologist, 22(2), 3-18.         [ Links ]

3 Krasnogorsky, N.I. (1925). The conditioned reflexes and children’s neuroses. American Journal Diseases of Children, 30, 753-768.

4 Linwick, D.C. & Overmier, J.B. (2006). Associatively activated representations of food events resemble food-outcome expectancies more closely than food-based memories. Learning & Behavior, 34(1), 1-12.        [ Links ]

5 Logan, C.A. (2005). The legacy of Adolf Meyer’s comparative approach: Worcester rats and the strange birth of the animal model. Integrative Physiological & Behavioral Science, 40(4), 169-181.

6 Masserman, J.H. (1943). Behavior and neurosis: An experimental psychoanalytic approach to psychobiologic principles. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.         [ Links ]

7 Mineka, S., Davidson, M., Cook, M. & Keir, R. (1984). Observational conditioning of snake fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93(4), 355-372.        [ Links ]

8 Mineka, S. & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: It’s not what you thought it was. American Psychologist, 61(1), 10-26.

9 Mowrer, O.H. (1947). On the dual nature of learning a re-interpretation of “conditioning” and “problem-solving“. Harvard Educational Review, 17, 102-148.

10 Overmier, J.B. & Leaf, R.C. (1965). Effects of discriminative Pavlovian fear conditioning upon previously or subsequently acquired avoidance responding. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 60(2), 213-217.        [ Links ]

11 Overmier, J.B. & Seligman, M.E.P. (1967). Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance responding. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63(1), 28-33.        [ Links ]

12 Overmier, J.B., Bull, J.A. & Trapold, M.A. (1971). Discriminative cue properties of different fears and their role in response selection in dogs. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 76(3), 478-482.        [ Links ]

13 Seligman, M.E.P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. Oxford, England: W.H. Freeman.        [ Links ]

14 Shenger-Krestinikova, N.R. (1921). [Differentiation of visual stimuli and their limits in the visual analyzer of the dog]. Izvestiya Petrogradskogo Nauchnogo Instituta Imeni P.F. Lesgafta, 3, 1-41.        [ Links ]

15 Siegel, S., Baptista, M., Kim, J.A., McDonald, R.V. & Weise-Kelly, L. (2000). Pavlovian psychopharmacology: The associative basis of tolerance. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 8(3), 276-293.        [ Links ]

16 Skinner, B.F. (1961). Cumulative record. NY: Appleton Century Crofts.        [ Links ]

17 Solomon, R.L. (1980). The opponent-process theory of acquired motivation: The costs of pleasure and the benefits of pain. American Psychologist, 35(8), 691-712.        [ Links ]

18 Tinklepaugh, O.L. (1928). An experimental study of representative factors in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 8(3), 197-236.        [ Links ]

19 Trapold, M.A. & Overmier, J.B. (1972). The second learning process in instrumental learning. In A.H. Black & W.F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current theory and research (pp. 427-452). NY: Appleton Century Crofts.        [ Links ]

20 Volpicelli, J.R., Ulm, R.R., Altenor, A. & Seligman, M.E. (1983). Learned mastery in the rat. Learning and Motivation, 14(2), 204-222.        [ Links ]

21 Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Oxford, England: Stanford University Press.        [ Links ]

 

 

Department of Psychology - Elliott Hall - University of Minnesota - 75 E. River Rd. - Minneapolis, MN - USA, 55455.

Fecha de recepción: 25 de septiembre de 2007
Fecha de aceptación: 5 de noviembre de 2007