SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.9 número3O declínio do nome-do-pai: violência e transgressão na passagem do séculoHelena Antipoff: um marco na história da psicologia em Minas Gerais índice de autoresíndice de assuntospesquisa de artigos
Home Pagelista alfabética de periódicos  

Temas em Psicologia

versão impressa ISSN 1413-389X

Temas psicol. vol.9 no.3 Ribeirão Preto dez. 2001

 

Pesquisa sobre expertise: perspectivas e limitações1

 

Research on expertise: perspectives and limitations

 

 

Afonso Galvão

Universidade Católica de Brasília

Endereço para correspondência

 

 


RESUMO

Diferenças entre o conhecimento de experts e não expertise em qualquer domínio é algo que sempre desafiou tanto cientistas quanto público em geral. Pesquisas têm indicado que o desempenho superior do expert pode ser explicado a partir de algumas características básicas de seu comportamento. Por exemplo, experts demonstram melhor memória para material significante do que não-experts e representam problemas de modo mais elaborado, empregando diferentes estratégias para resolvê-los. Este trabalho apresenta dados de algumas pesquisas recentes na área de expertise que tratam sobre os aspectos comuns do ambiente no qual a expertise se desenvolve, o conceito e importância do estudo individual deliberado para o alcance da expertise e fatores motivacionais que sustentam o aprendizado de longa duração em áreas como música instrumental, física, tênis e literatura. São mencionadas três gerações de modelos que tentam explicar a expertise. A conclusão oferece uma pequena análise das implicações da pesquisa sobre expertise para estudos da criatividade e educação como um todo, bem como sobre suas limitações e perspectivas futuras.

Palavras-chave: expertise, estudo deliberado, aprendizagem, modelos cognitivos, talento.


ABSTRACT

Differences between expert and non-expert knowledge in any domain has challenged both researchers and the general public. Research suggests that experts' superior performance capacity can be explained in terms"of some basic features of their behaviour. For example, experts show better memory than non experts for significant material and represent problems in a more elaborate manner, using different strategies to solve them. This paper reviews research in the area of expertise concerning the environment in which it develops, the concept and role of deliberate practice to achieve expertise, as well as the motivational factors that support long-term learning in areas such as instrumental music performance, tennis and literature. Three generations of models that try to explain expertise are mentioned. The conclusion offers a brief analysis on the implications of expertise research to domains such as creativity and education as a whole, and about its limits and future research perspectives.

Key words: expertise, deliberate practice, learning, cognitive models, talent.


 

 

Texto completo disponível apenas em PDF.

Full text available only in PDF format.

 

 

REFERENCIAS BIBLIOGRÁFICAS

Adelson, B. (1984). When novices surpass experts: The difficulty of a task may increase with expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory and cognition, 10, 483-495         [ Links ]

Anderson, J. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 396-406.         [ Links ]

Anderson, J. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Harvard: Harvard University Press.         [ Links ]

Anderson, J. (1987). Skill acquisition. Psychological Review, 94, 192-210.         [ Links ]

Anderson, J. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.         [ Links ]

Anderson, J.; Boyle, C. e Reiser, B. (1985). Intelligent tutoring systems. Science, 228, 456-462.         [ Links ]

Anderson, J.; Greeno; J.; Kline, P. e Neves, D. (1981). Acquisition of problem solving skill. Em J. Anderson (org.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 127-168). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.         [ Links ]

Bandura, A. e Schunk, D. (1981). Cultivating competence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 586-598         [ Links ]

Bhaskar, R. e Simon, H. (1977). Problem solving in semantically rich domains: An example of engineering thermodynamics, Cognitive Science, 1, 193-215.         [ Links ]

Brian, W. L. e Harter, N. (1897). Studies in the physiology and psychology of the telegraphic language. Psychological Review, 4, 27-53.         [ Links ]

Brian, W. L. e Harter, N. (1899). Studies on the telegraphic language: The acquisition of a hierarchy of habits. Psychological Review, 6, 345-375.         [ Links ]

Ceci, S. e Liker, J. (1986). A day at the races: A study of IQ, expertise, and cognitive complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 255-266.         [ Links ]

Chase, W. G. e Simon, H. A. (1973). The mind's eye in chess. Em W.G. Chase (org.), Visual information processing (pp. 156-168). New York: Academic Press.         [ Links ]

Chi, M. e Ceci, S. (1987). Content knowledge: Its role, representation and restmcturing in memory development. Advances in Child Development, 20, 91-142.         [ Links ]

Chi, M.; Feltovich, P. e Glaser, R. (1981). Categorisation and representation of physics problems by expert and novices. Cognitive Sciences, 5, 121-152.         [ Links ]

Corno, L. (1989). Self-regulated learning: A volitional analysis. Em B. Zimmerman e D. Schunk (org.), Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theory, research and practice. New York: Springer.         [ Links ]

Cotrell, G. (1985). Connectionist parsing, Proceedings. VII Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 201-211). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.         [ Links ]

Curtis, B. (1986). By the way, did anyone study any real programmers? Em E. Soloway e S. Iyengar (org.). Empirical studies of programmers. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.         [ Links ]

DeGroot, A. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. Paris: Mouton (Originalmente publicado em 1946).         [ Links ]

DeGroot, A. (1966). Perception and memory versus thought. Em B. Kleinmuntz (org.), Problem solving. New York: John Wiley.         [ Links ]

Ericsson, K. e Smith, J. (1994). Prospects and limits of the empirical study of expertise. Em K. A. Ericsson e J. Smith (orgs.). Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 1-38). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.         [ Links ]

Ericsson, K. e Staszewsky, J. (1989). Skilled memory and expertise: Mechanisms of exceptional performance. Em D. Klahr e K. Kotovsky (orgs.). Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 235-267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.         [ Links ]

Ericsson, K. A.; Tesch-Romer; C. e Kramp, R. (1990). The role of practice and motivation in the acquisition of expert-level performance in real life. Em M. Howe (org.), Encouraging the development of exceptional skills and talents (pp. 109-131). Leicester: British Psychological Society.         [ Links ]

Ericsson, K. A.; Tesch-Romer, C. e Kramp, R. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363-406.         [ Links ]

Frensch, P. e Sternberg, R. (1989). Expertise and intelligent thinking: When is it worse to know better. Em R. Sternberg (org.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (V.5, pp.157-181). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.         [ Links ]

Galton, F. (1979). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Julian Friedman (Originalmente publicado em 1869).         [ Links ]

Galvao, A. (2000a). Practice in orchestral life: An exploratory study of string players ' learning processes. PhD Thesis. Reading University, UK.         [ Links ]

Galvao, A. (2000b). Learning and the orchestra. Em C. Woods, G.Luck, R. Brochard, F. Seddon, e J. A.Sloboda (orgs.), Proceedings. VI International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (pp. 223-235). Keele, UK: Keele University. Department of Psychology.         [ Links ]

Galvao, A. e Kemp, A. (1999) Kinaesthesia and instrumental music performance: Some implications. Psychology of Music Journal, 29, 129-137.         [ Links ]

Glaser, R. (1996). Changing the agency for learning: Acquiring expert performance. Em K. A. Ericsson (org.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts, sciences, sports and games (pp. 303-312). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.         [ Links ]

Green, A. e Gilhooly, K. (1992). Empirical advances in expertise research. Em M. Keane e K. Gilhooly (org.), Advances in the psychology of thinking. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.         [ Links ]

Gustin, W. (1985). The development of exceptional research mathematicians. Em B. Bloom (org.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 270-33 \). New York: Balantine Books.         [ Links ]

Hallam, S. (1997). Approaches to instrumental music practice of experts and novices: Implications for education. Em H. Jorgensen e A. Lehmann (orgs.), Does practice make perfect? current theory and research on instrumental music practice (pp. 264-285). Oslo: NMH - Publikasjoner.         [ Links ]

Hatano, G. e Inagaki, K. (1986). Two courses of expertise. Em H. Stevenson, H. Azuma, e K. Hakuta (orgs.). Child development and education in Japan (pp. 262-272). San Francisco: Freeman.         [ Links ]

Hayes, J. (1981). The complete problem-solver. Philadelphia: Franklin Institute Press         [ Links ]

Hecht, H. e Proffitt, D. (1995). The price of expertise: Effects of experience on the water-level task. Psychological Science, 6, 90-95.         [ Links ]

Holyoak, K. (1994). Symbolic connectionism: Third generation theories. Em K. A. Ericsson e J. Smith (orgs.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 301-329). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.         [ Links ]

Howe, M. (1989). Fragments ofgenius: The strange feats of idiot savants. London: Routledge.         [ Links ]

Howe, M. (1996). Childhood and early lives of geniuses. Em K. A. Ericsson (org.), The road to excellence: The acquisition ofexpert performance in the arts, sciences, sports and games (pp. 255-270). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.         [ Links ]

Howe, M.; Davidson, J. e Sloboda, J. (1998). Innate talent: Reality or myth. Behavioural and Brain Science, 2, 399-442.         [ Links ]

Kemp, A. (1996). The musical temperament: Psychology and personality of musicians. Oxford: Oxford University Press.         [ Links ]

Kemp, A. (1997). Individual differences in musical behaviour. Em D. Hargreaves e A. North (orgs.). The social psychology of music (pp. 24-45). Oxford: Oxford University Press.         [ Links ]

Kintsch, W. (1994). Text comprehension, memory and learning. American Psychologist, 49, 294-302.         [ Links ]

Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional mediators of cognitive-behaviour consistency: Self-regulatory processes and action versus state orientation. Em J. Kuhl e J. Beckman (orgs.), Action control: From cognition to behaviour. New York: Springer-Verlag.         [ Links ]

Kurtz, B. e Borkowski, J. (1984). Children's metacognition: Exploring relations among knowledge, process, and motivational variables. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 335-354.         [ Links ]

Larkin, J. (1981). Enriching formal knowledge: A model for learning to solve textbook physics problems. Em J. Anderson (org.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.         [ Links ]

Larkin, J.; McDermott, J.; Simon; D. e Simon, H. (1980). Expert and novice performance in solving physics problems. Science, 208, 1335-1342.         [ Links ]

Larkin, J.H. (1983). The role of problem-representation in physics. Em N.J. Hillsdale (org.), Mental models (pp. 122-139). New Jersey: Erlbaum.         [ Links ]

Lehman, H. (1953). Age and achievement. Princeton: Princeton University Press.         [ Links ]

Logan, G. (1985). Skill and automaticity: Relations, implications and future directions. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 367-386.         [ Links ]

Monsaas, J. (1985). Learning to be a world-class tennis player. Em B. Bloom (org.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 211-269). New York: Balantine Books        [ Links ]

Newell, A. (1973). Production systems: Models of control structures. Em W. Chase (org.), Visual information processing (pp. 32-57). New York: Academic Press.         [ Links ]

Newell, A.; Shaw, J. e Simon, H. (1958). Elements of a theory of human problem solving. Psychological Review, 65, 151-166.         [ Links ]

Newell, A. e Simon, H. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Clifs: Prentice-Hall.         [ Links ]

Owen, E. e Sweller, J. (1985). What do students learn while solving mathematics problems? Journal of Educational Psychology, 77,272-284.         [ Links ]

Pirolli, P. e Anderson, J. (1985). The role of learning from examples in the acquisition of recursive programming skills. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 240-272.         [ Links ]

Rosenbloom, P. e Newell, A. (1986). The chunking of goal hierarchies: A generalised model of practice. Em R. Michalski e J. Mitchell (orgs.), Machine learning II: An artificial intelligence approach. Los Altos, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.         [ Links ]

Scardamalia, M. e Bereiter, C. (1994). Literate expertise. Em K. A. Ericsson e J. Smith (orgs.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 172-194). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.         [ Links ]

Schoenfeld, A. e Herrmann, D. (1982). Problem perception and knowledge structure in expert and novice mathematical problem solvers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory and cognition, 8, 484-494.         [ Links ]

Schunk, D. (1984). The self-efficacy perspective on achievement behaviour. Educational Psychologist, 19, 199-218.         [ Links ]

Seidenberg, M. e McClelland, J. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of work recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523-568.         [ Links ]

Shastri, L. e Ajjanagadde, V. (1990). From simple associations to systematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables and dynamic bindings. Pennsylvania: Computer and Information Science Department, University of Pennsylvania (Technical Report MS-CIS-90-05).         [ Links ]

Shiffrin, R. e Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127-190.         [ Links ]

Simon, H. e Chase, W. (1973). Skills in chess. American Scientist, 61, 394-403.         [ Links ]

Simonton, D. (1996). Creative expertise: A life-span developmental perspective. Em K. Ericsson (org.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports and games (pp. 227-254). New Jersey: LEA.         [ Links ]

Sloboda, J. (1985). The musical mind: The cognitive psychology of music. Oxford: Oxford University Press.         [ Links ]

Sloboda, J. (1996). The acquisition of musical expertise: Deconstructing the "talent" account of individual differences in musical expressivity. Em K.A. Ericsson (org.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports and games (pp. 107-126). New Jersey: LEA.         [ Links ]

Sloboda, J.; Davidson, J. e Howe, M. (1994). Is everyone musical? The Psychologist, 7, 349-354.         [ Links ]

Sloboda, J.; Davidson, J.; Howe, M. e Moore, D. (1996). The role of practice in the development of performing musicians. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 287-309.         [ Links ]

Sosniak, L. (1985). Learning to be a concert pianist. Em B. S. Bloom (org.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 19-67). New York: Ballantine Books.         [ Links ]

Sosniak, L. (1990). The tortoise, the hare, and the development of talent. Em M. Howe (org.), Encouraging the development of exceptional skills and talents (pp. 149-164). Leicester: British Psychological Society.         [ Links ]

Squire, L. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195-231.         [ Links ]

Sternberg, R. (1996). Costs of expertise. Em K. Ericsson (org.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports and games (pp. 347-354). New Jersey: LEA.         [ Links ]

Sweller, J.; Mawer, R. e Ward, M. (1983). Development of expertise in mathematical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 639-661.         [ Links ]

Thiede, K. e Dunlosky, J. (1999). Toward a general model of self-regulated study: An analysis of selection of items for study and self-paced study time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1024-1037.         [ Links ]

Thomas, J.; Iventosch, L. e Rohwer, W. (1987). Relationships among student characteristics, study activities, and achievement as a function of course characteristics. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 12, 344-364.         [ Links ]

Wagner, R. e Sternberg, R.(1986). Tacit knowledge and intelligence in the everyday world. Em R. Sternberg e R. Wagner (orgs.), Practical intelligence (pp. 51-83). New York: Cambridge University Press.         [ Links ]

Zimmerman, B. (1985). The development of "intrinsic" motivation: A social learning analysis. Annals of Child Development 2, 117-160.         [ Links ]

Zimmerman, B. (1989). A social cognitive view of self-regulated academic learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 329-339.         [ Links ]

Zimmerman, B. e Ringle, J. (1981). Effects of model persistence and statements of confidence on children's self-efficacy and problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 485-493.         [ Links ]

 

 

Endereço para correspondência:
Condomínio Vivendas Bela Vista, mod. R, casa 13,
CEP: 73.070-018, Sobradinho - DF

Recebido em: 29/11/2001
Aceito em: 25/04/2003

 

 

1. Trabalho apresentado no Simpósio Criatividade e Expertise: fatores facilitadores a seu desenvolvimento, XXXI Reunião Anual de Psicologia da Sociedade Brasileira de Psicologia, Rio de Janeiro - RJ, outubro de 2001

Creative Commons License