SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.16 número3Mindfulness e inteligência emocional na prevenção de recaídas em pessoas em tratamento: uma revisãoIntervenciones basadas en mindfulness para adultos y ancianos con trastornos mentales: una revisión integradora índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

SMAD. Revista eletrônica saúde mental álcool e drogas

versión On-line ISSN 1806-6976

SMAD, Rev. Eletrônica Saúde Mental Álcool Drog. (Ed. port.) vol.16 no.3 Ribeirão Preto jul./set. 2020

http://dx.doi.org/10.11606/issn.1806-6976.smad.2020.168328 

REVIEW ARTICLE

 

Mindfulness and emotional regulation: a systematic literature review*

 

 

Liana Santos Alves PeixotoI,II; Sônia Maria Guedes GondimI

IUniversidade Federal da Bahia, Instituto de Psicologia, Salvador, BA, Brazil
IIScholarship holder at the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES), Brazil

Corresponding author

 

 


ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: This systematic literature review study sought to analyze the relationship between mindfulness and emotional regulation.
METHOD: Several national and international databases were consulted and 60 articles published in the period from 2009 to 2019 were selected for analysis.
RESULTS: In conceptual terms, mindfulness is predominantly considered as a trait, and less as a state or skill. The results of empirical studies suggest that mindfulness is associated with the use of adaptive emotional regulation strategies favoring healthy psychic functioning. The limits and contributions of this review are also pointed out.
CONCLUSION: It is concluded that there is a need for further studies that consider the procedural aspect of mindfulness and that make it possible to capture the subjective experience of the practice, considering the prevalence of quantitative studies that used self-report scales.

Descriptors: Mindfulness; Literature Review; Emotions; Mental Health.


 

 

Introduction

Over the past three decades, there has been a notable growth in the scientific interest in the benefits promoted by the practice of mindfulness(1). Diverse empirical evidence points out that this practice can allow people to deal with their emotions in a more adaptive manner(2-3). Associated with mindfulness, the emotional regulation process has also been extensively investigated(4), bringing out evidence that both contribute to mental health and personal well-being(4-11).

The growing interest in exploring the role of mindfulness in the emotional regulation process is due to its effect in reducing the negative response to stress and in improving emotional management processes(12), enhancing the effect on psychological well-being(13-14). Higher levels of mindfulness have been associated with more adaptive emotional regulation processes in clinical and non-clinical populations(7, 15). However, the mechanisms of this relationship have not yet been widely understood(7, 11, 16).

One of the difficulties is the operationalization of the mindfulness(17) construct. The authors highlight three types of studies most found in the scientific literature on mindfulness: i) studies based on interventions (such as the use of the Mindfulness Based Stress reduction - MBSR program or full attention exercises), ii) comparison of mindfulness practitioners and non-practitioners, and iii) studies on the dispositional trait of mindfulness. This variability in studies is attributed to the different forms for the operationalization of mindfulness.

In the field of Psychology the term mindfulness has been used in three ways, with possible impacts on how the concept is operationalized in the empirical studies: i) characteristic or trait: stable quality that differs between the individuals, ii) momentary state: which can be induced; and iii) ability developed through the intervention: which involves learning by means of formal and informal practices. The diverse evidence on the effects of mindfulness differs depending on how it is operationalized(18). There is discussion in the literature(19) that some authors conceive it as a one-dimensional construct(20), while others see it as multi-factorial(21-22). There is also criticism on the trend to define and operate this construct as a stable trait, which departs from the definition of Buddhist texts that value its procedural and dynamic aspect(23).

Taking as a base the pointed out aspects, it was decided to carry out a systematic literature review in order to identify how mindfulness has been operationalized in the empirical studies and to characterize the nature of the relationships between mindfulness and the emotional regulation process in the national and international literature over the last ten years. Systematic reviews are important methodological resources in a context of accelerated growth of the scientific production. They allow scientists to answer questions about the quality and quantity of what is being produced in a specific scientific field, assisting researchers in the critical analysis of the accrued knowledge and in the elaboration of more promising research projects by taking into account empirical evidence and theoretical bases pointed out as more consistent(24-25).

It is expected that the results and the analysis carried out from this systematic review will contribute to offer a clearer picture of the operationalization and current state of the scientific production concerning the relationship between mindfulness and emotional regulation. It is also expected to contribute to the improvement of studies in this area and to offer information to subsidize intervention programs based on mindfulness.

 

Method

Systematic reviews are secondary studies conducted from published primary studies(24). The steps to carry out a literature review are the following: definition of the research question; search for evidence (definition of terms or keywords and search strategies); selection of studies (inclusion or exclusion in view of the established criteria); evaluation of the methodological quality of the study (by reading the articles), and presentation of the results (elaboration of an abstract on the selected studies and articulation with the research question)(25).

The guiding questions for this literature review were the following: "What are the empirical relationships between mindfulness and emotional regulation available in the national and international literature?" and "In what way has mindfulness been operationalized in these empirical studies?" The established time frame was from 2009 to 2019, and this period was chosen because it made it possible to analyze both more recent publications (last five years) and previous studies, broadening the understanding of the relationship between the constructs.

Initially, between November and December 2019, a search was conducted in Google Academic and in all the databases available in the CAPES (Commission for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel) Journal Portal for scientific articles on mindfulness and emotional regulation. The search was also conducted in specific databases such as Scientific Electronic Library Online (Scielo), Latin American and Caribbean Literature in Health Sciences (Literatura Latino-Americana e do Caribe em Ciências da Saúde, Lilacs) and the Research Gate website.

The criteria established for the selection of scientific articles were as follows: empirical articles, published from 2009 to 2019, using combinations of the keywords mindfulness, atenção plena, regulação emocional, emotional regulation and emotion regulation. The Boolean operator "and" was used so that both keywords were included in the searches. The search was limited to articles published in Portuguese, English and Spanish, whose participants have been adults. The databases were configured to search the words "título" and "assunto" in the CAPES Portal and "title" and "keywords" in Google Academic and in the other databases. Through ResearchGate, access was obtained to articles whose full text was not available in the databases consulted. The exclusion criteria were removal of theses, theoretical articles, review or meta-analysis articles, editorials, comments on articles, books and book chapters.

A total of 762 articles were found in the CAPES Journal Portal databases, 350 documents in Google Academic, one article in Scielo, one article in ResearchGate and no article in Lilacs, totaling 1, 114 documents. From the reading of the title, the first selection of articles that met the inclusion criteria was started. In cases where the title, abstract or keywords were not sufficient to decide on the inclusion of the article, the entire study was read. Duplicate documents were also excluded. At the end of this first stage, 283 texts were selected, 236 identified in the databases available in the CAPES journal platform, 45 in Google Academic, one in Scielo and one in Research Gate. The second and last stage of the selection of empirical articles was made by reading the abstracts of these articles. The established inclusion criteria were taken into account, excluding those articles that made reference to very specific subject matters, which would hinder drawing general conclusions from relations between mindfulness and emotional regulation. Therefore, very specific articles on psychopathologies, training focused on compassion or on the cognitive aspect, chronic pain, decision-making processes, target population of children, adolescents and older adults, among others, were removed from the final list of articles. There were 60 articles left for analysis, 45 in the CAPES portal, 14 in Google Academic, and one in Research Gate.

The databases and the respective number of articles identified for reading were as follows: Springer (n=19), Elsevier (n=18), PubMed (n=9), Routledge (n=4), American Psychological Association (n=3), Ebsco (n=2), Semantic Scholar (n=2), Wiley Periodicals (n=2) and Australian Psychological Society (n=1).

The selected articles were read in full and a synthesis of the main information, such as year, authorship, study objective and conclusions, was carried out. From reading the texts, a categorization was made regarding the operationalization of mindfulness using the direct information of the text or from the way in which the author treated the construct (trait, state or ability). In addition, the studies were categorized according to their objective, through analysis of the thematic content(26-27) where the studies were grouped by thematic similarity or proximity, since the data analysis of a literature review must observe patterns and carry out comparisons among the studies, grouping them into analytical categories(28). The categories were elaborated in an inductive manner, after reading the articles selected by the first author and orientation by the second author. These categories were created trying to answer the questions of this literature review.

 

Results

Initially, the general characteristics of the reviewed studies will be described and, later, an analysis of their objectives and main results will be presented. It is noted that most publications occurred in the last five years (2015 to 2019), with 37 publications in this period (61.6%) and 23 between 2009 and 2014 (38.4%).

The reviewed studies were conducted in several countries: United States of America (n=15), China (n=4), Germany (n=2), Iran (n=2), Israel (n=2), Switzerland (n=2), Austria (n=1), Germany and Austria (n=1), Brazil (n=1), South Korea (n=1), India (n=1), Italy (n=1), Japan (n=1), United Kingdom (n=1), Singapore (n=1) and Sweden (n=1). The study locus was not mentioned in 23 studies.

In relation to the participants, there was a prevalence of division into two groups, with 26 studies being conducted with adult individuals from the general population and 26 with students. Five studies had a clinical sample (individuals under treatment or seeking treatment for anxiety and depression disorders) and three merged clinical and non-clinical participants.

Regarding the operationalization of mindfulness, in 38 studies mindfulness was defined as a trait (stable individual characteristic), in eight as a state (momentary state induced during data collection), and in nine as an ability (developed through an intervention program based on mindfulness or brief training). Three studies addressed mindfulness both as a state and as a trait and two treated it as a state and as an ability, as described in Figure 1.

Among the most commonly used instruments to measure the constructs under analysis, the use of self-reporting scales was predominant. On mindfulness, the Five-Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ(21);n=25) and Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS(20); n=19) were prevalent and, in relation to the measurements of emotional regulation, the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS(36); n=16), Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS(37); n=15) and Emotional Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ(38); n=14) stood out.

As for the studies, they were organized in five categories (Figures 2 to 5) by similarity related to the research focus. Most of the studies (n=19) sought to identify the effects of mindfulness and of emotional regulation strategies on the emotional response and on the regulation of emotions (Figure 2). The most specific focus of the desired relationship was: i) mindfulness and specific strategies, example: cognitive reevaluation and rumination (n= 5); ii) mindfulness and the response and involvement of the individuals with emotional stimuli (n=3); iii) mindfulness and the capacity for emotional differentiation in the regulation of emotions (n=3); iv) the effects of a program based on mindfulness on the emotional experience (n =3); and v) how the dimensions of these constructs interrelate and impact on improvements in health and well-being (n=3). In summary, the results of these studies indicate positive effects of mindfulness on the emotional regulation process (more adaptive, more flexible, greater use of reevaluation, fewer negative thoughts, less stress), with repercussions on health and on well-being(4, 7, 31, 34, 39-45).

Contrary to the strong assumption that mindfulness practitioners would be less affected by emotional stimuli, in one of the studies, experienced practitioners had high emotional engagement with the tasks proposed when compared to non-practitioners, as well as faster recovery than the latter(32). The emotional stimulus was induced from the presentation of films, after which the response time was measured in the recall of a memory with emotional content opposite to that induced (e.g., sad movie - happy memory). In another study, it was proposed to investigate reactivity and emotional recovery to affective stimuli and it was identified that individuals who presented higher mindfulness scores had a rapid recovery after negative stimuli induced by images with affective content, but without decreased reactivity (measured by the subjective evaluation of the images)(46).

Positive associations were identified between mindfulness and the ability to differentiate emotions. Higher mindfulness scores were shown to be associated with greater capacity for emotional differentiation and less emotional difficulty (self-reported emotional deregulation and less emotional lability)(47). The ability to differentiate negative emotions was associated with higher mindfulness scores, with repercussions on the adaptive response to stress(48). Another study goes on to investigate if, in addition to emotional differentiation, the ability to differentiate context assessment (interpretation of reality) would be a mediator of the relationship between mindfulness and the capacity for emotional differentiation(49). The results suggest that individuals with lower mindfulness scores experience undifferentiated emotional states not only because of the inability to regulate emotions, but also because of their interpretation of the world (different and competing evaluations), therefore with emphasis also on the cognitive aspect.

In addition, some studies investigated dimensions of mindfulness in association with emotional regulation strategies. The "describe" and "non-reactivity" dimensions were associated with greater use of reevaluation and acceptance and with lesser use regarding suppression of emotions; observation with more rumination; and non-judgment with less rumination(50). In another study it was identified that some factors associated with mindfulness (description, acting consciously) predict less reactivity to stress and anguish(12). There was also a discussion on the overlapping of mindfulness and emotional regulation, considering their factors and emphasizing the mediators: clarity, managing negative emotions, detachment and rumination in the relationship between mindfulness and mental health(1).

The studies that included other constructs to deepen the understanding of the relationship between mindfulness and emotional regulation (n=15), presented in Figure 3, make reference to affects (n=4), mood (n=3), behavior motivation (n=2), work memory (n=2), attachment (n=2), self-efficacy (n=1) and social connection (n=1). The results provide evidence of positive effects of mindfulness on affects, mood, memory and social connection(16, 51-57). Another result indicates that affect and self-efficacy would mediate the relationship between mindfulness and emotional regulation, with positive effects on satisfaction with life(10, 58). Additionally, mindfulness, emotional regulation and attachment would be correlated(29, 59). Finally, the studies indicate that mindfulness relates to the behavior inhibition system, which provides for greater vigilance with behavior inhibition, and problems with psychic functioning (anxiety). That is, individuals who are more sensitive to the behavioral inhibition system have more difficulty in regulating emotions, partly because they have difficulties in using important dimensions of mindfulness, such as to acting consciously, not judging and not being reactive(60), and that acceptance and the facets of mindfulness (observation and non-reactivity) would cushion the influence of the behavioral inhibition system on psychological suffering(61).

In turn, in the studies where the relationships between mindfulness, emotional regulation and psychic functioning (n=14) were evaluated, shown in Figure 4, it is observed that, in seven of them, psychological distress or dysfunctions such as anxiety and depression were emphasized. In five, focus was given to well-being and mental health and, in one article, both dysfunctions and health and well-being were investigated.

Among the results of these studies, we highlight the findings that emotional regulation would function as a mediator in the relationship between mindfulness and dysfunctions or mental health(13-14, 17, 62-64) with the exception of a study that proposed a model where emotional regulation would facilitate mindfulness, with consequences for health and well-being(65). It is also noted that mindfulness and emotional regulation would be predictors of healthy psychic functioning(66-68) and problematization if non-reactivity, acceptance, non-judgment and acting consciously would be relevant in the relationship between mindfulness, emotional regulation and psychic functioning(69-72).

In the studies where neurobiological factors involved in the practice of mindfulness (n=10) were addressed, gathered in Figure 5, specific regions of the brain were investigated, such as amygdala and frontal cortex (n=3), as well as the brain effects of mindfulness-based techniques or interventions (n=3), the brain effects of different emotional regulation strategies (n=3), affective reactivity to emotional stimuli (n=1) and neural mechanisms of generalized anxiety (n=1). The results achieved with its performance indicated changes in the brain regions and neural mechanisms involved in emotional processing (amygdala and pre-frontal cortex) with optimization of the emotional regulation process(33, 35, 73-79). One study tested the moderating effect of mindfulness in the response to emotional stimuli (images with emotional content), assuming that the reactivity to stimuli would be lower, since mindfulness facilitates a receptive and non-reactive posture to external and internal stimuli(18). However, this study found no empirical evidence that dispositional mindfulness moderates the individual reaction (assessed with physiological and psychological measures) to emotional stimuli.

Finally, in the two studies where comparisons of meditation modalities and/or stress control techniques were made, associations were found between mindfulness meditation and the twin hearts meditation** (n=1); and between mindfulness, passive progressive muscle relaxation,*** and lovingkindness meditation****(n=1). The results indicated that mindfulness-based meditation favors a posture of greater decentralization (a more detached way of relating to one's thoughts) than other techniques(80). In a study, however, it was pointed out that the twin hearts meditation was more effective for the emergence of positive affects than mindfulness-based meditation(81). It is important to consider the objectives of each type of meditation in order to understand the scope of these results. The twin hearts and loving-kindness meditations induce positive emotions trough directing them to the entire humanity, and the progressive muscle relaxation directs the focus of attention towards relaxing the body. The practices of mindfulness, in their turn, only suggest that the individual perceives the emotions and sensations found in the body, regardless of what they are. Therefore, the results of each type of meditation are consistent with the objectives they are proposed for, as seen in the results: mindfulness promoting greater decentralization than loving-kindness meditation and progressive muscle relaxation, while the twin hearts meditation promotes more positive emotions than mindfulness.

 

Discussion

This review sought to answer two questions: (i) identify how mindfulness has been operationalized in empirical studies, and (ii) characterize the nature of the relationships between mindfulness and the emotional regulation process in the national and international literature over the last ten years. In relation to the first question, we observed the presence of three main operationalizations of mindfulness in the researched studies: trait, state and ability. This discussion exists in the literature(18, 82), where it is emphasized that the way in which mindfulness is operationalized is crucial to the investigative process of this construct. These authors point out that there are different ways to investigate the mindfulness construct, considering it as a trait or characteristic that varies among the individuals, as a temporary state that can be induced and manipulated in the laboratory for a short period of time, and also as an ability that can be developed through mindfulness-based interventions(83). In this review, there was predominance of the conception of mindfulness as a trait, followed by as an ability and, finally, as a temporary state.

The operationalization of the construct is linked to a conception of mindfulness and to the way it is measured. Some studies have found evidence of the relationship between mindfulness treated as a state or as an ability to decrease reactivity to emotional stimuli(18). On the other hand, there are inconclusive results when mindfulness is treated as a trait. In some studies included in this review, for example, dispositional mindfulness was not associated with reduced affective reactivity during the visualization of emotional stimuli(18, 46).

In the studies of this review, there was also no evidence on the use of qualitative methodologies to learn the subjective aspect of the mindfulness experience, but the frequent use of scales to measure this construct. Despite the scientific value of self-reporting measures that provide information on the individuals' self-perception, there is the problem of social desirability and inaccuracy in the response due to the individuals' lack of full knowledge on their own states and behaviors(23). In case of the scales, there can be lack of understanding of the items, resulting in different interpretations of their meanings for the researcher and the participants(84) or even among participants with different levels of familiarity with the concept(19, 23).

Western people are relatively inexperienced with the concept and experience of mindfulness, which impacts on the understanding of the formulated items(72). On the other hand, in defense of the mindfulness scales, several studies have identified their good psychometric properties measured by internal consistency and predictive validity(19), and predictive power of psychological problems(20), also pointing out positive correlations between high scores and practice time(21) and higher scores after the intervention(20).

Thus, it becomes relevant to consider the operationalization of the concept, since the two most cited measures in this review for assessing mindfulness have different factorial structures. The MAAS scale(20) has a one-dimensional structure to measure the awareness of the moment in the everyday experiences, whereas FFMQ(21) is multi-dimensional (five factors), consisting of observing, describing, not judging the inner experience, acting consciously, and not being reactive to the inner experience.

This variability in the operationalization of mindfulness can represent an obstacle to understanding the construct(19, 23) and to conducting research studies that may come to bring out robust evidence about its effectiveness for the mental health of the individuals, as the measures may be evaluating different aspects, even with overlaps in relation to other constructs such as emotional regulation.

In a study presented in this review, the hypothesis of overlap between the mindfulness factors and the concept of emotional regulation was corroborated(36), even if they are different processes(1). In examining these overlaps, the authors identified that there are some common factors, namely: "acceptance of the internal experience", "recognition of the internal experience" and "behavior in the presence of unpleasant internal experiences". On the other hand, the mindfulness factors of "attention in the present" and "acting consciously" were identified as specific to mindfulness, without overlapping with emotional regulation factors. Finally, in this study it was also revealed that the constructs under analysis can contribute or facilitate the expression of each other.

Seeking to respond to the existing interactions between mindfulness and emotional regulation, the analysis of the reviewed studies reasserted the conception found in the literature that mindfulness and emotional regulation are constructs associated with psychic functioning and mental health, and that they seek to understand the mechanisms that are at the basis of these relationships. In addition, some research studies included their interaction with other constructs of Psychology, such as affects, mood, self-efficacy, and social connection. The studies also included a neurobiological approach to identify brain changes through the practice of mindfulness.

The analyzed constructs may come to contribute to or facilitate the expression of each other(1), which was also pointed out by other reviewed studies(65). The view that the relationship between emotional regulation and mindfulness is considered pertinent can be bidirectional(29), and that mindfulness-based interventions can provide better emotional regulation, while individuals with adaptive emotional regulation strategies have higher levels of mindfulness.

There is great variety in the literature regarding the interactions between mindfulness and emotional regulation, with studies where relations of mediation, moderation, prediction, associations or correlations are indicated. Furthermore, in the selected articles there was no consensus that mindfulness would be considered an emotional regulation strategy, as only six of the analyzed studies conceptualize mindfulness in such a way.

There is still the view that mindfulness is not just a specific emotional regulation strategy(31). Mindfulness plays a broader role in the processes of emotional regulation, affecting the most central regulatory stages. This view is shared in review(2, 5) and theoretical(85) articles. Mindfulness can promote flexibility in the cognitive evaluation by increasing interoceptive attention, which impacts on the process of emotional regulation(40).

Thus, understanding the relationship between mindfulness and emotional regulation in empirical studies requires returning to these concepts and to their operationalization, as well as investing in scientific efforts for a clearer and more precise delineation of the possible connections between these constructs that have been shown to be essential for the mental health and psychic functioning of the individuals.

 

Conclusion

This literature review provides an overview of the existing discussions in the scientific field about the relationship between mindfulness and emotional regulation, in addition to discussing operational aspects related to the conception of mindfulness. Regarding the operationalization of mindfulness, the following is observed: predominance in the studies that treat it as a trait, the use of self-reporting measures, and the prevalence of quantitative analyses. Studies with a qualitative approach, which allow for a better understanding of the subjective experience, were not identified in the sample of this review. Some authors also argue about the overlapping of factors of the mindfulness constructs and emotional regulation, which hinders conceptualizing and understanding their relationships, in addition to the lack of consensus regarding the factors that make up mindfulness.

The review showed that, in fact, mindfulness and emotional regulation are interrelated. In the studies analyzed, it was observed that mindfulness has repercussions in the process for choosing emotional strategies by (i) facilitating the use of more adaptive strategies such as cognitive reevaluation; (ii) reducing the use of dysfunctional strategies such as rumination and suppression; and (iii) giving more flexibility in the choice of strategies since the individual is more present and receptive to the demands of the context. Additionally, mindfulness would be related to the process of effective emotional regulation, as it would promote (i) good capacity for emotional differentiation; (ii) rapid emotional recovery after negative stimuli; and (iii) high emotional engagement with emotional stimuli. In addition, it is worth noting that the results of the studies were inconclusive in relation to the investigation of the relationships between emotional reactivity, mindfulness, and the emotional regulation process.

Emotional regulation was also characterized as an important mediator in the relationship between mindfulness and mental health, although there are other models that conceive mindfulness as the mediator of the relationship between emotional regulation and mental health. There was also a discussion that mindfulness and emotional regulation are predictors of mental health and that the dimensions of mindfulness are related to the emotional regulation process and to mental health, favoring it, with the exception of the observation dimension that, in isolation, is associated with higher anxiety levels.

It was verified that the relationship between mindfulness and emotional regulation contributes with positive effects on other constructs such as affects, mood, memory, and social connection; in addition to being related to concepts such as self-efficacy and attachment. And, finally, that mindfulness contributes to changes in the brain regions and neural mechanisms involved in optimizing the process for regulating the emotions.

It was thus identified that there are positive repercussions of high levels of mindfulness and adaptive emotional regulation strategies with healthy psychic functioning, but the mechanisms underlying this process require more research. Another aspect is that the conceptual imprecision and the measures used to evaluate these constructs make it difficult to identify the relationships between them, often indicating overlap. The studies have advanced in the investigation of constructs and an important aspect they highlighted is the need to explore the processes of emotional regulation in the intervention programs, given the connection between mindfulness and the regulation of the emotions for psychological well-being.

In most of the studies identified in this review, a cross-sectional methodology was used, which limits conclusions about causality or temporal precedence. Thus, an observed gap was the need for more longitudinal studies. In addition, we highlight the use of other measures (physiological, for example) and strict control conditions (control group) to determine the specificity of the effects of mindfulness, thus indicating a need for further experimental and intervention studies.

However, there are some clear limitations in this review because, even though a consultation was carried out in various databases, some journals relevant to the area may not have been contemplated. Another methodological weakness of this study is that the selection of articles and data analysis was performed by only one researcher, without validation by an external judge or group of independent reviewers. It was also decided to delimit the review to empirical studies, excluding review and meta-analysis studies that could have added relevant information for understanding the relationship between mindfulness and the emotional regulation process. Although some review studies have been consulted, they have not been consulted in a systematic way, as was the case with empirical studies.

It is therefore concluded that future research studies must consider and explain the conceptual and operational aspects of mindfulness and emotional regulation for creating greater conceptual clarity since, in this review, it was verified that this is an aspect that needs better understanding. The results indicated in this literature review can contribute to a better understanding of the advances in the scientific field that relates mindfulness with emotional regulation processes, since these constructs have an impact on the healthy psychic functioning of the individuals.

 

References

1. Coffey KA, Hartman M, Fredrickson BL. Deconstructing mindfulness and constructing mental health: understanding mindfulness and its mechanisms of action. Mindfulness. 2010;1(4):235-53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-010-0033-2        [ Links ]

2. Roemer L, Williston SK, Rollins LG. Mindfulness and emotion regulation. Curr Opin Psychol. 2015;3:52-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.02.006        [ Links ]

3. Zarotti N, Povah C, Simpson J. Mindfulness mediates the relationship between cognitive reappraisal and resilience in higher education students. Personal Individ Differ. 2020;156:109795. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.109795        [ Links ]

4. Igna R, Ştefan S. Comparing Cognitive Strategies in the Process of Emotion Regulation. J Evid-Based Psychother. 2015;15(2):251-66. Available from: http://web-a-ebscohost.ez10.periodicos.capes.gov.br/ehost/detail/detail?vid=0&sid=ca0e6d39-7061-44f6-bc04-b691798a632c%40sessionmgr4006&bdata=Jmxhbmc9cHQtYnImc2l0ZT1laG9zdC1saXZl#AN=110321514&db=aph        [ Links ]

5. Chambers R, Gullone E, Allen NB. Mindful emotion regulation: An integrative review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009;29(6):560-72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.005        [ Links ]

6. Corcoran KM, Farb N, Anderson A, Segal ZV. Mindfulness and Emotion Regulation: Outcomes and Possible Me diat ing Me chanisms. In: Kring AM, Sloan DM, organizadores. Emotion regulation and psychopathology: a transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2010. p. 339-55.         [ Links ]

7. Erisman SM, Roemer L. A preliminary investigation of the effects of experimentally induced mindfulness on emotional responding to film clips. Emotion. 2010;10(1):72. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017162        [ Links ]

8. Hölzel BK, Lazar SW, Gard T, Schuman-Olivier Z, Vago DR, Ott U. How Does Mindfulness Meditation Work? Proposing Mechanisms of Action From a Conceptual and Neural Perspective. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011;6(6):537-59. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611419671        [ Links ]

9. Jin Y, Zhang M, Wang Y, An J. The relationship between trait mindfulness, loneliness, regulatory emotional self-efficacy, and subjective well-being. Personal Individ Differ. 2020;154:109650. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.109650        [ Links ]

10. Luberto CM, Cotton S, McLeish AC, Mingione CJ, O'Bryan EM. Mindfulness Skills and Emotion Regulation: the Mediating Role of Coping Self-Efficacy. Mindfulness. 2014;5(4):373-80. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0190-6        [ Links ]

11. Teper R, Segal ZV, Inzlicht M. Inside the Mindful Mind: How Mindfulness Enhances Emotion Regulation Through Improvements in Executive Control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2013;22(6):449-54. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413495869        [ Links ]

12. Bullis JR, Bøe HJ, Asnaani A, Hofmann SG. The benefits of being mindful: Trait mindfulness predicts less stress reactivity to suppression. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2014;45(1):57-66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.07.006        [ Links ]

13. Cheung RYM, Ng MCY. Mindfulness and Symptoms of Depression and Anxiety: the Underlying Roles of Awareness, Acceptance, Impulse Control, and Emotion Regulation. Mindfulness. 2019;10(6):1124-35. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-018-1069-y        [ Links ]

14. Freudenthaler L, Turba JD, Tran US. Emotion Regulation Mediates the Associations of Mindfulness on Symptoms of Depression and Anxiety in the General Population. Mindfulness. 2017;8(5):1339-44. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0709-y        [ Links ]

15. Coffey KA, Hartman M. Mechanisms of Action in the Inverse Relationship Between Mindfulness and Psychological Distress. Complement Health Pract Rev. 2008; 13(2):79-91. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533210108316307        [ Links ]

16. McLaughlin LE, Luberto CM, O'Bryan EM, Kraemer KM, McLeish AC. The indirect effect of positive affect in the relationship between trait mindfulness and emotion dysregulation. Personal Individ Differ. 2019;145:70-4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.03.020        [ Links ]

17. Parmentier FBR, García-Toro M, García-Campayo J, Yañez AM, Andrés P, Gili M. Mindfulness and Symptoms of Depression and Anxiety in the General Population: The Mediating Roles of Worry, Rumination, Reappraisal and Suppression. Front Psychol. 2019; 10:1-10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00506        [ Links ]

18. Cosme D, Wiens S. Self-Reported Trait Mindfulness and Affective Reactivity: A Motivational Approach Using Multiple Psychophysiological Measures. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(3):1-23. https://doi.org/ 10.1371/journal.pone.0119466        [ Links ]

19. Sauer S, Walach H, Schmidt S, Hinterberger T, Lynch S, Büssing A, et al. Assessment of Mindfulness: Review on State of the Art. Mindfulness. 2013;4(1):3-17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0122-5        [ Links ]

20. Brown KW, Ryan RM. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;84(4):822. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822        [ Links ]

21. Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using Self-Report Assessment Methods to Explore Facets of Mindfulness. Assessment. 2006;13(1):27-45. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191105283504        [ Links ]

22. Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, Carlson L, Anderson ND, Carmody J, et al. Mindfulness: A Proposed Operational Definition. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2004;11(3):230-41. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph077        [ Links ]

23. Grossman P, Van Dam NicholasT. Mindfulness, by any other name...: trials and tribulations of sati in western psychology and science. Contemp Buddhism. 2011;12(1):219-39. https://doi.org/10.1080/14639947.2011.564841        [ Links ]

24. Galvão TF, Pereira MG. Revisões sistemáticas da literatura: passos para sua elaboração. Epidemiol E Serviços Saúde. 2014;23(1):183-4. https://doi.org/10.5123/S1679-49742014000100018        [ Links ]

25. Sampaio R, Mancini M. Estudos de revisão sistemática: um guia para síntese criteriosa da evidência científica. Rev Bras Fisioter. 2007;11(1):83-9. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-35552007000100013        [ Links ]

26. Bardin L. Análise de conteúdo. Lisboa: Edições 70; 2010.         [ Links ]

27. Gondim SMG, Bendassolli PF. Uma crítica da utilização da análise de conteúdo qualitativa em psicologia. Psicol Estud. 2014;19(2):191-9. https://doi.org/10.1590/1413-737220530002        [ Links ]

28. Soares CB, Hoga LAK, Peduzzi M, Sangaleti C, Yonekura T, Silva DRAD. Integrative Review: Concepts And Methods Used In Nursing. Rev Esc Enferm USP. 2014;48(2):335-45. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0080-6234201400002000020        [ Links ]

29. Goodall K, Trejnowska A, Darling S. The relationship between dispositional mindfulness, attachment security and emotion regulation. Personal Individ Differ. 2012;52(5):622-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.12.008        [ Links ]

30. Remmers C, Topolinski S, Koole SL. Why Being Mindful May Have More Benefits Than You Realize: Mindfulness Improves Both Explicit and Implicit Mood Regulation. Mindfulness. 2016;7(4):829-37. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0520-1        [ Links ]

31. Alkoby A, Pliskin R, Halperin E, Levit-Binnun N. An eight-week mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) workshop increases regulatory choice flexibility. Emotion. 2018;19(4):593. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000461        [ Links ]

32. Greenberg J, Meiran N. Is Mindfulness Meditation Associated with "Feeling Less?" Mindfulness. 2014;5(5):471-6. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-013-0201-2        [ Links ]

33. Egan RP, Hill KE, Foti D. Differential effects of state and trait mindfulness on the late positive potential. Emotion. 2017;18(8):1128. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000383        [ Links ]

34. Garland E, Kiken L, Faurot K, Palsson O, Gaylord S. Upward Spirals of Mindfulness and Reappraisal: Testing the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory with Autoregressive Latent Trajectory Modeling. Cogn Ther Res. 2017;41(3):381-92. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-016-9768-y        [ Links ]

35. Doll A, Hölzel BK, Mulej Bratec S, Boucard CC, Xie X, Wohlschläger AM, et al. Mindful attention to breath regulates emotions via increased amygdala-prefrontal cortex connectivity. NeuroImage. 2016;134:305-13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.041        [ Links ]

36. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional Assessment of Emotion Regulation and Dysregulation: Development, Factor Structure, and Initial Validation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2003;14. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94        [ Links ]

37. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063        [ Links ]

38. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85(2):348. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348        [ Links ]

39. Arch JJ, Craske MG. Laboratory stressors in clinically anxious and non-anxious individuals: The moderating role of mindfulness. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(6):495-505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.02.005        [ Links ]

40. Garland EL, Hanley A, Farb NA, Froeliger B. State Mindfulness During Meditation Predicts Enhanced Cognitive Reappraisal. Mindfulness. 2015;6(2):234-42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-013-0250-6        [ Links ]

41. Kiken LG, Shook NJ. Does mindfulness attenuate thoughts emphasizing negativity, but not positivity? J Res Personal. 2014;53:22-30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.08.002        [ Links ]

42. Lalot F, Delplanque S, Sander D. Mindful regulation of positive emotions: a comparison with reappraisal and expressive suppression. Front Psychol. 2014; 5: 1-9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00243        [ Links ]

43. Prakash RS, Whitmoyer P, Aldao A, Schirda B. Mindfulness and emotion regulation in older and young adults. Aging Ment Health. 2017;21(1):77-87. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2015.1100158        [ Links ]

44. Robins CJ, Keng S, Ekblad AG, Brantley JG. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on emotional experience and expression: a randomized controlled. J Clin Psychol. 2012;68(1):117-31. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20857        [ Links ]

45. Sauer SE, Baer RA. Responding to Negative Internal Experience: Relationships Between Acceptance and Change-Based Approaches and Psychological Adjustment. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2009;31(4):378-86. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-009-9127-3        [ Links ]

46. Cho S, Lee H, Oh KJ, Soto JA. Mindful attention predicts greater recovery from negative emotions, but not reduced reactivity. Cogn Emot. 2017;31(6):1252-9. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1199422        [ Links ]

47. Hill CLM, Updegraff JA. Mindfulness and its relationship to emotional regulation. Emotion. 2012;12(1):81. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026355        [ Links ]

48. Fogarty FA, Lu LM, Sollers JJ, Krivoschekov SG, Booth RJ, Consedine NS. Why It Pays to be Mindful: Trait Mindfulness Predicts Physiological Recovery from Emotional Stress and Greater Differentiation among Negative Emotions. Mindfulness. 2015;6(2):175-85. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-013-0242-6        [ Links ]

49. Tong EMW, Keng S-L. The Relationship Between Mindfulness and Negative Emotion Differentiation: a Test of Multiple Mediation Pathways. Mindfulness. 2017;8(4):933-42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0669-7        [ Links ]

50. Iani L, Lauriola M, Chiesa A, Cafaro V. Associations Between Mindfulness and Emotion Regulation: the Key Role of Describing and Nonreactivity. Mindfulness. 2019;10(2):366-75. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-018-0981-5        [ Links ]

51. Brockman R, Ciarrochi J, Parker P, Kashdan T. Emotion regulation strategies in daily life: mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression. Cogn Behav Ther. 2017;46(2):91-113. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2016.1218926        [ Links ]

52. Dubert CJ, Schumacher AM, Locker L, Gutierrez AP, Barnes VA. Mindfulness and Emotion Regulation among Nursing Students: Investigating the Mediation Effect of Working Memory Capacity. Mindfulness. 2016;7(5):1061-70. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0544-6        [ Links ]

53. Kaiser R, Andrews-Hanna J, Metcalf C, Dimidjian S. Dwell or Decenter? Rumination and Decentering Predict Working Memory Updating After Interpersonal Criticism. Cogn Ther Res. 2015;39(6):744-53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9697-1        [ Links ]

54. Keng S-L, Robins CJ, Smoski MJ, Dagenbach J, Leary MR. Reappraisal and mindfulness: A comparison of subjective effects and cognitive costs. Behav Res Ther. 2013;51(12):899-904. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2013.10.006        [ Links ]

55. Quaglia JT, Goodman RJ, Brown KW. From mindful attention to social connection: The key role of emotion regulation. Cogn Emot. 2015;29(8):1466-74. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.988124        [ Links ]

56. Snippe E, Dziak JJ, Lanza ST, Nyklíček I, Wichers M. The Shape of Change in Perceived Stress, Negative Affect, and Stress Sensitivity During Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction. Mindfulness. 2017;8(3):728-36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0650-5        [ Links ]

57. Zhang Q, Wang Z, Wang X, Liu L, Zhang J, Zhou R. The Effects of Different Stages of Mindfulness Meditation Training on Emotion Regulation. Front Hum Neurosci. 2019; 13:208. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00208        [ Links ]

58. LeBlanc S, Uzun B, Aydemir A. Structural relationship among mindfulness, reappraisal and life satisfaction: The mediating role of positive affect. Curr Psychol 2019; 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00383-x        [ Links ]

59. Pepping CA, Davis PJ, O'Donovan A. Individual differences in attachment and dispositional mindfulness: The mediating role of emotion regulation. Personal Individ Differ. 2013;54(3):453-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.10.006        [ Links ]

60. Reese ED, Zielinski MJ, Veilleux JC. Facets of mindfulness mediate behavioral inhibition systems and emotion dysregulation. Personal Individ Differ. 2015;72:41-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.008        [ Links ]

61. Hamill TS, Pickett SM, Amsbaugh HM, Aho KM. Mindfulness and acceptance in relation to Behavioral Inhibition System sensitivity and psychological distress. Personal Individ Differ. 2015;72:24-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.007        [ Links ]

62. Desrosiers A, Vine V, Klemanski DH, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Mindfulness and emotion regulation in depression and anxiety: common and distinct mechanisms of action. Depress Anxiety. 2013;30(7):654-61. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22124        [ Links ]

63. MacDonald HZ, Baxter EE. Mediators of the Relationship Between Dispositional Mindfulness and Psychological Well-Being in Female College Students. Mindfulness. 2017;8(2):398-407. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0611-z        [ Links ]

64. Pepping CA, O'Donovan A, Zimmer‐Gembeck MJ, Hanisch M. Is emotion regulation the process underlying the relationship between low mindfulness and psychosocial distress? Aust J Psychol. 2014;66(2):130-8. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12050        [ Links ]

65. Mandal SP, Arya YK, Pandey R. Mindfulness, Emotion Regulation, and Subjective Well-Being: Exploring the Link. SIS J Proj Psychol Ment Health. 2017;24:57-63. Available from: https://web.a.ebscohost.com/abstract?direct=true&profile=ehost&scope=site&authtype=crawler&jrnl=09716610&AN=121809598&h=
TjlogRqxnLuQLcz2ELIsb0BO5Gn8L2wK3FYY2fKwkTV%2f%2bRc4NxEdZchNxwOKqHtlqY58zYJ9sleWV%2f7sgdT8cQ%3d%3d&crl=f&resultNs=
AdminWebAuth&resultLocal=ErrCrlNotAuth&crlhashurl=login.aspx%3fdirect%3dtrue%26profile%3dehost%26scope%3dsite%26authtype%
3dcrawler%26jrnl%3d09716610%26AN%3d121809598
        [ Links ]

66. Mahmoudzadeh S, Mohammadkhani P, Dolatshahi B, Moradi S. Prediction of psychological well-being based on dispositional mindfulness and cognitive emotion regulation strategies in students. Practice Clin Psychol. 2015;3(3):195-202. Available from: http://jpcp.uswr.ac.ir/browse.php?a_id=212&sid=1&slc_lang=en        [ Links ]

67. Mitmansgruber H, Beck TN, Höfer S, Schüßler G. When you don't like what you feel: Experiential avoidance, mindfulness and meta-emotion in emotion regulation. Personal Individ Differ. 2009;46(4):448-53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.11.013        [ Links ]

68. Shafieetabar M, Akbari-Chermahini S. The Role of Mindfulness and Emotional Regulation in Predicting Depression and Anxiety in University Students. Int J Behav Sc, 2017;11(3):101-6. Available from: https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/The-Role-of-Mindfulness-and-Emotional-Regulation-in-Shafieetabar-Akbari-Chermahini/7ac259d05398432ca8750526a153a7662902407e        [ Links ]

69. Curtiss J, Klemanski DH, Andrews L, Ito M, Hofmann SG. The conditional process model of mindfulness and emotion regulation: An empirical test. J Affect Disord. 2017;212:93-100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.01.027        [ Links ]

70. Desrosiers A, Vine V, Curtiss J, Klemanski DH. Observing nonreactively: A conditional process model linking mindfulness facets, cognitive emotion regulation strategies, and depression and anxiety symptoms. J Affect Disord. 2014;165:31-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.024        [ Links ]

71. Kotsou I, Leys C, Fossion P. Acceptance alone is a better predictor of psychopathology and well-being than emotional competence, emotion regulation and mindfulness. J Affect Disord. 2018;226:142-5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.047        [ Links ]

72. Ostafin BD, Brooks JJ, Laitem M. Affective Reactivity Mediates an Inverse Relation Between Mindfulness and Anxiety. Mindfulness. 2014;5(5):520-8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-013-0206-x        [ Links ]

73. Hölzel BK, Hoge EA, Greve DN, Gard T, Creswell JD, Brown KW, et al. Neural mechanisms of symptom improvements in generalized anxiety disorder following mindfulness training. NeuroImage Clin. 2013;2:448-58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2013.03.011        [ Links ]

74. Kral TRA, Schuyler BS, Mumford JA, Rosenkranz MA, Lutz A, Davidson RJ. Impact of short- and long-term mindfulness meditation training on amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli. NeuroImage. 2018;181:301-13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.013        [ Links ]

75. Lutz J, Herwig U, Opialla S, Hittmeyer A, Jäncke L, Rufer M, et al. Mindfulness and emotion regulation-an fMRI study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9(6):776-85. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst043        [ Links ]

76. Modinos G, Ormel J, Aleman A. Individual differences in dispositional mindfulness and brain activity involved in reappraisal of emotion. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2010;5(4):369-77. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq006        [ Links ]

77. Opialla S, Lutz J, Scherpiet S, Hittmeyer A, Jäncke L, Rufer M, et al. Neural circuits of emotion regulation: a comparison of mindfulness-based and cognitive reappraisal strategies. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2015;265(1):45-55. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-014-0510-z        [ Links ]

78. Xiao Q, Zhao X, Bi G, Wu L, Zhang H, Liu R, et al. Alterations of Regional Homogeneity and Functional Connectivity Following Short-Term Mindfulness Meditation in Healthy Volunteers. Front Hum Neurosci. 2019;13,1-12 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00376        [ Links ]

79. Zhang W, Ouyang Y, Tang F, Chen J, Li H. Breath-focused mindfulness alters early and late components during emotion regulation. Brain Cogn. 2019;135:103585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2019.103585        [ Links ]

80. Feldman G, Greeson J, Senville J. Differential effects of mindful breathing, progressive muscle relaxation, and loving kindness meditation on decentering and negative reactions to repetitive thoughts. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(10):1002-11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.006        [ Links ]

81. Valim CPRAT, Marques LM, Boggio PS. A Positive Emotional-Based Meditation but Not Mindfulness-Based Meditation Improves Emotion Regulation. Front Psychol. 2019;10:1-12 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00647        [ Links ]

82. Davidson RJ. Empirical explorations of mindfulness: Conceptual and methodological conundrums. Emotion. 2010;10(1):8. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018480        [ Links ]

83. Heppner WL, Spears CA, Vidrine JI, Wetter DW. Mindfulness and Emotion Regulation. In: Ostafin BD, Robinson MD, Meier BP, organizators. Handbook of mindfulness and self-regulation. New York; Heidelberg Dordrecht; London: Springer; 2015. p. 107-20. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_9        [ Links ]

84. Kohlsdorf M, Da Costa ÁL Junior. O autorrelato na pesquisa em psicologia da saúde: desafios metodológicos. Psicol Argum. 2017;27(57):131. https://doi.org/10.7213/rpa.v27i57.19763        [ Links ]

85. Hayes AM, Feldman G. Clarifying the Construct of Mindfulness in the Context of Emotion Regulation and the Process of Change in Therapy. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2004;11(3):255-62. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph080        [ Links ]

 

 

Corresponding Author:
Liana Santos Alves Peixoto
E-mail: liana.sap@gmail.com

Received: Mar 3rd 2020
Accepted: July 6th 2020

 

 

Author's Contribution
Study concept and design: Liana Santos Alves Peixoto and Sonia Maria Guedes Gondim. Obtaining data: Liana Santos Alves Peixoto. Data analysis and interpretation: Liana Santos Alves Peixoto and Sonia Maria Guedes Gondim. Obtaining financing: Liana Santos Alves Peixoto. Drafting the manuscript: Liana Santos Alves Peixoto and Sonia Maria Guedes Gondim. Critical review of the manuscript as to its relevant intellectual content: Sonia Maria Guedes Gondim.
All authors approved the final version of the text.
Conflict of interest: the authors have declared that there is no conflict of interest.
* This article refers to the call "Mindfulness and other contemplative practices".
* Paper extracted from doctoral dissertation "Mindfulness, emotional regulation and academic career: an investigation with postgraduate students", presented to Instituto de Psicologia, Universidade Federal da Bahia, Salvador, BA, Brazil.
** The Twin Hearts meditation was created by Grandmaster Choa Kok Sui and consists of visualizing planet Earth and directing positive emotions to the entire humanity.
*** Passive progressive muscle relaxation is a technique where the individuals are guided to perceive sensations of tension in each part of the body and then allow these muscles to relax completely
**** The loving-kindness meditation is based on the Buddhist tradition and consists of creating feelings of social connection and compassion for oneself and for others, through the repetition of phrases like: "I live in safety, I live happily" and gradually extend this to significant others and unknown people: "May you live safely, may you live happily"

Creative Commons License